INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS ON THE INTERNET: RISKY ACTIONS AND THE PERCEPTION OF SERIOUSNESS IN ADOLESCENCE
University of the Basque Country - UPV/EHU, Department of Didactics and School Organization (Spain)
Received November 2024
Accepted March 2025
Abstract
The use of the Internet and social media by adolescents to socialize with other people entails both potential risks and benefits. But what role is played by the perception of the seriousness of risky actions that adolescents take as part of their interpersonal relationships over the Internet? The aim of this study is to analyze the frequency of risky behaviors on social media and the perception of the seriousness by the adolescent population in their interpersonal relationships. This study considers a sample of 2529 adolescents in Spain, Italy and Greece between the ages of 10 and 17 years. The results indicate that a proportional relationship exists between the increase in age and the performance of risky actions. Girls tend to take fewer risky actions and their perception of the seriousness is greater compared to boys of the same age. The correlational analysis shows that risky actions increase as the perception of seriousness decreases, a trend that persists when controlling for the age variable. Along these lines, it is evident that the perception of seriousness is a variable that plays an important role in the regulation of risky behaviors in adolescents in the digital environment. This finding may be key for digital literacy processes, since it suggests that the educational approaches must focus on strengthening the perception of the seriousness associated with risky actions. This implies promoting the critical analysis of the risky nature of these actions and reflecting on their impact, both on a personal and a community level.
Keywords – Internet, Social media, Risk analysis, Risk perception, Adolescence.
To cite this article:
Etxague-Goia, I., & Cuervo-Sánchez, S.L. (2025). Interpersonal relationships on the internet: Risky actions and the perception of seriousness in adolescence. Journal of Technology and Science Education, 15(2), 529-542. https://doi.org/10.3926/jotse.3207 |
----------
-
-
1. Introduction
-
Social media plays a fundamental role for the adolescent community, in so far as they are used to relate to the group of peers, among other activities. However, these relationships entail risks and opportunities, according to the behaviors in which adolescents engage on social media and the perception of the seriousness of said actions or behaviors.
Depending on the social, economic, cultural and developmental context of each person, adolescents of both genders may generate adaptive or maladaptive strategies in the management of social media that will facilitate or complicate their individual development. Among the former are self-control, the prioritization of healthy activities and the exploration of varied off-line alternatives. The latter include the avoidance of parental control, at-risk behaviors, legitimizing the abusive use of the Internet and the acceptance of aggressive behaviors as natural (Arab & Díaz, 2015).
This situation leads us to pose questions about the frequency with which the adolescent population takes risks related to Internet and digital device use, taking into account the conducts and behaviors in which they engage on social media with other people. Along the same lines, questions are raised regarding the perception of the seriousness of these activities and behaviors. In this research, the results of an intercultural study are presented, with 2529 adolescents who are asked about their behaviors and perceptions of the seriousness of those behaviors on social media.
1.1. Paradoxes in Internet Use to Socialize in Adolescence
In the field of communications technology, the emergence of social media and Internet use to establish interpersonal relationships has generated different paradoxes in terms of its risks and benefits. These paradoxes are the subject of debates and broad-reaching research, particularly when the focus is on how the adolescent population uses social media, digital devices and the Internet.
With regard to the interpersonal relationships that adolescents establish through social media, of particular interest is the research by Livingstone and Stoilova (2021) on the classification of risks by conducts, behaviors, contact and contracts that can be generated in these virtual spaces. In the case of risks associated with contact, there is the risk of interaction with adults who might cause them harm, exemplified by the acceptance of friendship with a stranger or setting up a meeting with someone they have previously met on the Internet.
Within the cyber-communication processes facilitated by social media, trust is a fundamental aspect of accepting a friendship request, and in turn, it can be an explanation of the motivation that leads people to take this type of risk with persons they do not know. The trust that leads someone to accept an online friendship may be based on common interests, interactions that take place over a long period of time or social validation (Ellison, Steinfield & Lampe, 2007).
The building of friendship is a progressive process that ranges from superficial interactions to revealing personal aspects (Taylor & Altman, 1987). According to previous research, adolescents who play online video games tend to accept invitations from other players on the same platform, which can result in friendships that last over a long period of time (Carras, Van Rooij, Van de Mheen, Musci, Xue & Mendelson, 2017).
Likewise, social media play a crucial role for those adolescents who are seeking to become part of communities with similar interests, such as those who belong to LGTBIQ+ groups, ethnic groups, political ideologies and different cultures. From this perspective, these digital platforms emerge as places of refuge, emotional support, validation and mutual support for young people, where they establish relationships of friendship with people they have previously met on the Internet (Brandt & Carmichael, 2020).
Participation in the social media can be considered beneficial, since they promote the creation and strengthening of communities based on shared experiences and interests, significantly contributing to the development and consolidation of personal identity. This phenomenon, as indicated by Brandt and Carmichael (2020), underscores the positive potential of social media in the learning and growth process of adolescents.
From another perspective, the way in which the adolescent population manages relationships with strangers on social media may be influenced by cultural and gender-related factors. Studies indicate that adolescent girls, for example, are more likely to establish emotional connections with friends, while boys tend to focus on shared interests, such as video games or sports (Svensson, Johnson & Olsson, 2022). However, in research on the problematic use of social media by persons with low self-esteem, depression or high levels of anxiety, it has been seen that interaction with strangers can increase these problems, especially in adolescence, when they are faced with situations of cyber-bullying or grooming (Kumar & Goldstein, 2020). Similarly, as part of the trust paradox, we find the vulnerability to manipulation and deceit that the underage population may experience, as they may be less skillful in identifying the warning signs in online interactions, when they believe that they form part of a relationship of friendship. Along the same lines, the perception of the seriousness of this type of risky behavior can be diminished, since these are relationships established outside the off-line world.
On the other hand, accepting the friendship of strangers can lead to undesired posting of inappropriate content or contact with potentially harmful persons for children and adolescents, which is an added risk associated with accepting friendship with someone you have previously met on the Internet.
In addition, social media relationships often involve overexposure, where adolescents reveal feelings, thoughts and emotions that can place them at risk; these are thus examples of risky conducts. This conduct can be related to the revealing of information to strengthen the bonds of friendship. At the same time, it can be related to online risks associated with the loss of the sense of privacy and narcissism reflected in self-exhibition.
However, privacy is a concept that has been influenced by the structure of social media and how it works. According to Marwick and Boyd (2014), the social media offers tools for introducing oneself and getting to know other people, pushing underage minors to reevaluate their individual desires for privacy in an interconnected society, where sharing has become a requirement for participation. This dynamic decreases the perception of risk, since these conducts are normalized by social pressure and the popularity of practices such as providing immediate updates on daily activities, like exercising or visiting a store.
Along these lines, the paradox once again arises between the risks and the opportunities offered by social media. From the perspective of information exchange, revealing personal information is essential for establishing relationships of friendship; however, this also poses certain risks, as this information can be used by companies for commercial purposes and also facilitate fraud or cyber-bullying among individuals (Zani, Norman & Ghani, 2022).
With regard to behaviors on social media, adolescents may also relate to one another through behaviors associated with bullying and exclusion. Livingstone and Stoilova (2021) refer to these as behavioral risks. These behaviors are characterized by the potential harm they can cause among peers and in other groups. According to Marwick (2021), bullying on social media acts as a means though which to impose a social order, often used by individuals who, thanks to their privileges in society, can engage in harassment without any major consequences and suppress the voices of minority groups or potentially vulnerable people. Accordingly, those who question or challenge the power structures in force are usually more vulnerable to being bullied by those who align themselves with the social norms. Those who experience situations of bullying suffer psychological harm that results in self-censure or isolation, which is related to the social regulation from those who hold power, a common phenomenon in adolescence.
Previous studies (Zhu, Huang, Evans & Zhang, 2021) indicate that the figures for cyber-bullying on an international level vary from 14% to 52%. In the case of Spain, a higher prevalence of victimization of 57.5% (Marco & Tormo-Irun, 2018) has been reported, with variations according to the socioeconomic and geographic context, as well as by age and gender. With regard to differences by gender, researchers have shown that women have a greater probability of becoming victims, especially through indirect behaviors like social exclusion or the spreading of rumors, while boys tend to be involved more often as aggressors (Kumar & Goldstein, 2020; Wright & Wach, 2020).
According to Marwick (2021), bullying on the Internet is more common for women and non-binary people. This study agrees with a study conducted by Pew Research Center (2018), who found that 15% of adolescent girls reported having been victims of cyber-bullying, as compared to 6% of boys. Furthermore, age plays an important role, as younger adolescents, between 12 and 14 years of age, are particularly vulnerable, due to their greater exposure time on the social media, which increases the possibility of being victims or perpetrators of bullying (Gómez-Tabares & Correa-Duque, 2022). This type of bullying has different characteristics from traditional bullying, such as the anonymity of the aggressor and the quick dissemination of hostile content, which can aggravate the psychological impact on the victims (Slonje & Smith, 2008).
In this same realm, we see the figure of the silent witnesses who, while not participating in the aggression directly, assume a position of apparent neutrality. However, their inaction acts as implicit support for the bully and negatively reinforces the experience of the victim. In this sense, Garandeau, Turunen, Saarento‑Zaprudin and Salmivalli (2023) indicate that increasing the defensive behavior of the observers or witnesses is a challenge. The fear of becoming the next victim prevents many potential defenders from taking action; furthermore, the presence of other people reduces the chances of anyone providing assistance, since it is assumed that someone else will take the responsibility of intervening. Lambe, Della‑Cioppa, Hong and Craig (2019) indicate that most classmates who witness situations of bullying fail to intervene, and only 17%-19% of the witnesses assume the role of a defender when the harassment occurs in person.
Recent research concludes that cyber-bullying, as well as passive attitudes towards aggression, have serious consequences on the mental health of minors, increasing the risk for anxiety, depression and suicidal thoughts (Ortega, Elipe, Moran-Merchán, Calmaestra & Vega, 2009), as well as generating stressful situations, isolation and helplessness (López‑Sáez, Pérez-Torres, Pastor, Lobato-Rincón, Thomas-Currás & Angulo-Brunet, 2024). Some studies have found that familiarity with risky actions reduces the perception of risk that the adolescent community has of them (Livingstone & Helsper, 2010), and that tolerance of violence is related to a cultural acceptance of stereotypical actions, which lessens the perception of the seriousness of the aggressive actions (for example, those performed by men) (Wang, Gao & Chen, 2023). However, studies on the perception of seriousness by the adolescent population regarding this type of bullying behavior are still scarce (Etxague-Goia, 2023).
In this sense, this research has two main objectives: firstly, to analyze the frequency of risky actions in virtual relations among adolescents according to gender and age, as well as the perceived seriousness of said actions in both variables. Secondly, the aim is to examine the relationship between engaging in risky actions and the perception of their seriousness, considering the influence of age on both aspects.
2. Methodology
2.1. Research Design
This research is a descriptive, quantitative, exploratory and correlational study. This approach made it possible to analyze the phenomena in question and identify the relationships among the different variables. An online questionnaire was used (White, Carey & Daily, 2001) and developed according to the guidelines proposed by Lumsden (2007) and Norman, Friedman, Norman and Stevenson (2001). This instrument has been used and validated in previous studies (Cuervo-Sánchez & Etxague, 2023; Lareki, Fraga-Varela & Martínez-de-Morentin, 2024; Martínez-de-Morentin, Lareki & Altuna, 2021).
2.2. Sample
The sample consists of 2529 adolescents, of whom 1262 are boys (49.9%), 1264 are girls (50.0%) and 3 were classified as other (0.1%), between 10 and 17 years of age and from seven different territories. Specifically, 38.4% come from the Autonomous Community of the Basque Country, 20.2% from Galicia, 15.4% from Navarre, 11.5% from Greece, 5.9% from Cantabria, 4.5% from Madrid and 4.0% from Italy. As far as the age of the users goes, the most numerous group was that of the 12-year-olds, who made up 20.0% of the participants, followed by the 13-year-olds (18.3%), 14-year-olds (17.5%) and 15-year-olds (16.4%). Users who are 11 years old represent 12.5% of the sample and those who are 10 years old accounted for 7.6%. The least represented groups are the 16-year-olds (5.5%) and 17-year-olds (2.1%).
2.3. Instrument and Data Analysis
The instrument used for data collection was an online questionnaire entitled “Anomia digital. Uso de las tecnologías digitales y comportamientos inadecuados” [Digital anomie. The use of digital technologies and inappropriate behaviors], which consisted of 39 items organized in five different dimensions: usage habits, contents and downloads, data management, relations and posts (Martínez-de-Morentin, Lareki, Altuna & Amenabar, 2018). In the protocol, participants were given a detailed explanation and their questions were answered about the meaning of the phrases and the scale used for each item. This study focuses on the dimension of relationships, and it includes 10 items:
Item 1: I accept friendship requests from people I do not know.
Item 2: I talk badly in public about a classmate.
Item 3: I exclude a classmate (from the group, I block them, I ignore them, etc.).
Item 4: I threaten classmates.
Item 5: I meet on the street with people that I have met through social media.
Item 6: I write lies about others.
Item 7: I make comments that promote bullying (insulting someone)
Item 8: I talk about very personal things (feelings, relationships, etc.) with people I do not know well.
Item 9: I spread rumors or criticisms that hurt others.
Item 10: I do not get involved when someone is being bullied.
Participants evaluated the frequency with which they performed the described actions, using a Likert-type scale with four options: (1) never, (2) rarely, (3) often, (4) always. In addition, they classified the seriousness of the actions according to three levels: (1) minor, (2) moderate, (3) serious. The Cronbach’s alpha value for the questionnaire as a whole is 0.665, while for the relationship dimension is 0.836. This reliability index is based on the scores for the questionnaire that consider the items of interest. A value higher than 0.6 generally indicates a reasonable internal consistency (Huh, Delorme & Reid, 2006; Malhotra, 2008).
In order to carry out the study, the administrative teams of the schools and the students were notified, who gave their approval. Informed consent was obtained from the students, according to the favorable decision of the Ethics Committee and the Organic Law 3/2018, of 5 December, on the Protection of Personal Data and the assurance of digital rights. The questionnaire was completed by the students during class time, under the supervision of an instructor from the respective schools.
The statistical program SPSS (version 24) was used to analyze the data. An analysis of the descriptive statistics was carried out, along with comparisons of the means for independent groups (t-test), the calculation of the effect size (Cohen’s d), an analysis of variance (ANOVA) and a correlation analysis.
3. Results
The results of the study are presented in this section. First, the statistics of risky actions related to the relationships are described (Table 1) and these actions are analyzed according to gender (Table 2) and age (Table 3); likewise, the correlation that exists between performing these actions and age is also shown (Table 4). Second, the perception of the risk associated with risky actions is analyzed (Table 5) according to gender (Table 6) and age (Table 7). Finally, the correlation between risky actions and the perception of the seriousness is shown (Table 8).
3.1. Risky Actions Associated with Relationships
The main results indicate that most of the adolescents rarely engage in risky behaviors on the Internet. 56.4% (N = 1426) never accept friendship requests from strangers, 78.2% (N = 1978) never say bad things publicly about a classmate. Threatening others or spreading lies is uncommon, with 87.5% (N = 2213) of the participants never performing these actions. However, 25.5% (N = 645) have rarely left out classmates, and 27.2% (N = 689) indicate that they rarely intervene when someone is being bullied.
|
Mean |
Standard deviation |
Never |
Rarely |
Often |
Always |
Total |
|||||
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
|||
I make comments that promote bullying (insulting someone) |
1.12 |
0.433 |
2241 |
88.6 |
183 |
7.2 |
28 |
1.1 |
23 |
0.9 |
2475 |
97.9 |
I write lies about others |
1.13 |
0.406 |
2213 |
87.5 |
228 |
9 |
26 |
1 |
13 |
0.5 |
2480 |
98.1 |
I threaten classmates |
1.14 |
0.463 |
2236 |
88.4 |
183 |
7.2 |
33 |
1.3 |
29 |
1.1 |
2481 |
98.1 |
Relationships: compound variable |
1.18 |
0.34151 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
I spread rumors or criticisms that hurt others |
1.19 |
0.495 |
2102 |
83.1 |
303 |
12 |
48 |
1.9 |
22 |
0.9 |
2475 |
97.9 |
I tell very personal things (feelings, relationships, etc.) to people I do not know well |
1.22 |
0.537 |
2039 |
80.6 |
355 |
14 |
61 |
2.4 |
27 |
1.1 |
2482 |
98.1 |
I meet on the street with people I have met on social media |
1.23 |
0.552 |
2053 |
81.2 |
327 |
12.9 |
74 |
2.9 |
29 |
1.1 |
2483 |
98.2 |
I talk badly in public about a classmate |
1.26 |
0.559 |
1978 |
78.2 |
419 |
16.6 |
63 |
2.5 |
30 |
1.2 |
2490 |
98.5 |
I leave out a classmate (out of a group, I block them, I ignore them, etc.) |
1.42 |
0.677 |
1664 |
65.8 |
645 |
25.5 |
128 |
5.1 |
46 |
1.8 |
2483 |
98.2 |
I accept friendship requests from people I do not know |
1.61 |
0.843 |
1426 |
56.4 |
726 |
28.7 |
211 |
8.3 |
126 |
5 |
2489 |
98.4 |
I do not get involved when someone is bullied |
1.63 |
0.867 |
1414 |
55.9 |
689 |
27.2 |
212 |
8.4 |
141 |
5.6 |
2456 |
97.1 |
Table 1. Risky actions in interpersonal relationships: descriptive statistics
Gender |
N |
Mean |
Standard deviation |
Mean standard error |
t |
Sig. (bilateral) |
Cohen’s D |
Boys |
1181 |
1.3253 |
0.39670 |
0.01154 |
4.727 |
0.000 |
0.194 |
Girls |
1199 |
1.2582 |
0.28607 |
0.00826 |
Table 2. Risky actions by gender
Analyzing risky actions by gender (Table 2), the results show a significant difference between boys and girls in terms of risky actions related to interpersonal relationships on the Internet. Boys showed a mean of 1.33, slightly higher than the girls, whose mean was 1.26. This difference, while small, is statistically significant (p. < 0.000), which suggests that boys tend to engage more frequently in this type of risky conduct. In addition, the responses by the boys show greater dispersion (standard deviation = 0.40) as compared to the girls (standard deviation = 0.29), which indicates greater variability in the behavior of the boys in this context. The effect size (d = 0.194) is weak (Cohen, 1988).
In terms of age (Table 3), we could say that as age increases, so does the mean for participation in risky actions, starting at 1.09 at age 10 years and reaching 1.51 at ages 16 and 17 years. This trend suggests that older adolescents tend to engage more in this type of behaviors. The analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicates that these differences are statistically significant (p. < 0.000), with an effect size (η2 = 0.110) that is moderate, nearing large. This suggests that as adolescents grow, their participation in risky behaviors related to relationships on the Internet increases.
Analyzing the correlation between age and risky actions associated with relationships (Table 4), a moderate positive correlation of 0.321 is observed between the variables of age and actions associated with relationships, with a level of significance lower than 0.001, which indicates that this correlation is statistically significant. In other words, as age increases, risky actions also tend to increase.
Age |
N |
M |
Standard deviation |
Standard error |
ANOVA |
||
F (between groups) |
Sig. (between groups) |
Eta squared |
|||||
10 |
182 |
1.0967 |
0.17365 |
0.01287 |
41.974 |
0.000 |
0.110 |
11 |
304 |
1.1280 |
0.21258 |
0.01219 |
|||
12 |
478 |
1.2232 |
0.29085 |
0.01330 |
|||
13 |
435 |
1.3340 |
0.36086 |
0.01730 |
|||
14 |
411 |
1.3521 |
0.34861 |
0.01720 |
|||
15 |
392 |
1.3796 |
0.35414 |
0.01789 |
|||
16 |
129 |
1.5147 |
0.42685 |
0.03758 |
|||
17 |
51 |
1.5137 |
0.57793 |
0.08093 |
|||
Total |
2382 |
1.2916 |
0.34685 |
0.00711 |
Table 3. Risky actions by age
|
Age |
Risky actions in relationships |
|
Age |
Pearson |
1 |
0.321** |
Sig. (bilateral) |
|
<0.001 |
|
N |
2528 |
2382 |
|
Risky actions in relationships |
Pearson |
0.321** |
1 |
Sig. (bilateral) |
<0.001 |
|
|
N |
2382 |
2383 |
|
**The correlation is significant at the level of 0.01 (bilateral). |
Table 4. Correlation between age and risky actions
3.2. Perception of the Seriousness of Actions Associated with Relationships
|
Mean |
Standard deviation |
Minor |
Mean |
Serious |
Total |
||||
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
N |
% |
|||
I leave out a classmate (out of a group, I block them, I ignore them, etc.) |
1.98 |
0.85 |
821 |
32.5 |
616 |
24.4 |
787 |
31.1 |
2224 |
88 |
I accept friendship requests from people I do not know |
2.01 |
0.822 |
749 |
29.6 |
730 |
28.9 |
766 |
30.3 |
2245 |
88.8 |
I tell very personal things (feelings, relationships, etc.) to people I do not know well |
2.12 |
0.844 |
673 |
26.6 |
606 |
24 |
932 |
36.9 |
2211 |
87.5 |
I talk badly in public about a classmate |
2.15 |
0.847 |
651 |
25.7 |
573 |
22.7 |
992 |
39.2 |
2216 |
87.6 |
I meet on the street with people I have met on social media |
2.19 |
0.869 |
666 |
26.3 |
465 |
18.4 |
1083 |
42.8 |
2214 |
87.5 |
I write lies about others |
2.2 |
0.858 |
637 |
25.2 |
498 |
19.7 |
1076 |
42.5 |
2211 |
87.4 |
I do not get involved when someone is bullied |
2.2 |
0.835 |
594 |
23.5 |
581 |
23 |
1027 |
40.6 |
2202 |
87.1 |
Perception of seriousness: compound variable |
2.2106 |
0.77279 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
I spread rumors or criticisms that hurt others |
2.23 |
0.855 |
610 |
24.1 |
476 |
18.8 |
1121 |
44.3 |
2207 |
87.2 |
I threaten classmates |
2.31 |
0.864 |
588 |
23.3 |
350 |
13.8 |
1275 |
50.4 |
2213 |
87.5 |
I make comments that promote bullying (insulting someone) |
2.31 |
0.873 |
603 |
23.8 |
309 |
12.2 |
1292 |
51.1 |
2204 |
87.1 |
Table 5. Perception of the seriousness of risky actions: descriptive statistics
The most pertinent results on the perception of risk (Table 5) show that the actions perceived as being the most serious are “threatening classmates” (N = 1275, 50.4%) and “making comments that promote bullying” (N = 1292 people, 51.1%), both with a mean of 2.31. Furthermore, 44.3% (N = 1121 people) consider the action “spreading rumors or criticisms that hurt others” to be serious, with a mean of 2.23. In contrast, the actions least perceived as serious are “excluding a classmate” (N = 787 people, 31.1%) and “accepting friend requests from strangers” (N = 766 people, 30.3%), with means of 1.98 and 2.01, respectively.
Gender |
N |
Mean |
Standard deviation |
Mean standard error |
t |
Sig. (bilateral) |
Cohen’s D |
Boys |
1036 |
2.0929 |
0.70513 |
0.02191 |
-4.992 |
0.000 |
-0.218 |
Girls |
1067 |
2.2455 |
0.69655 |
0.02132 |
Table 6. Perception of seriousness by gender
As far as the analysis by gender (Table 6), the results show a significant difference in the perception of seriousness associated with the actions related to interpersonal relationships on the Internet between boys and girls. Girls have a mean of 2.25, higher than the mean of 2.09 for boys, which indicates that they perceive a greater risk for these actions. This difference is statistically significant (p. < 0.000), even though the variability in the responses of both groups is similar, as indicated by the close standard deviations. Their effect size (d = 0.218) lies between weak (d = 0.20) and moderate (d = 0.50) (Cohen, 1988). These findings suggest that girls are more careful or aware of the risks in online relationships.
In terms of age (Table 7), it can be seen that the means of the perception of seriousness range from 2.04 for the 14-year-olds to 2.36 for the 11-year-olds, while the overall mean for all age groups is 2.17. The standard deviation of the responses indicates a variability that ranges from 0.63 to 0.80, suggesting that the perceptions are relatively consistent among the participants in each group, although with certain differences. The ANOVA analysis reveals an F value of 5.627, with a significance of p. < 0.000, which indicates that the differences in the perception of seriousness among the different age groups are statistically significant. However, their effect size is small (η2 = 0.018), which suggests that the influence of age on these differences is limited.
Age |
N |
M |
Standard deviation |
Standard error |
ANOVA |
||
F (between groups) |
Sig. (between groups) |
Eta squared |
|||||
10 |
165 |
2.2921 |
0.79786 |
0.06211 |
5.627 |
0.000 |
0.018 |
11 |
275 |
2.3582 |
0.72858 |
0.04394 |
|||
12 |
411 |
2.1538 |
0.75792 |
0.03739 |
|||
13 |
373 |
2.1458 |
0.67327 |
0.03486 |
|||
14 |
363 |
2.0457 |
0.66851 |
0.03509 |
|||
15 |
354 |
2.1814 |
0.63656 |
0.03383 |
|||
16 |
117 |
2.0821 |
0.63213 |
0.05844 |
|||
17 |
47 |
2.1085 |
0.65899 |
0.09612 |
|||
Total |
2105 |
2.1709 |
0.70455 |
0.01536 |
Table 7. Perception of seriousness by age
|
Age |
Perception of seriousness |
|
User age |
Pearson correlation |
1 |
-0.089 |
Sig. (bilateral) |
|
0.052 |
|
N |
2528 |
481 |
|
Perception of seriousness |
Pearson correlation |
-0.089 |
1 |
Sig. (bilateral) |
0.052 |
|
|
N |
481 |
481 |
Table 8. Correlation between age and the perception of the seriousness
The results in Table 8 show a weak negative correlation between age and the perception of the seriousness of risky actions, with a Pearson correlation coefficient of -0.089. However, this correlation suggests that the older the participants are, the more likely they are to perceive risky actions as being slightly less serious. However, the bilateral significance value (p = 0.052) indicates that this relationship is not statistically significant, as it exceeds the threshold of 0.05.
3.3. Correlation among Risky Actions, the Perception of Seriousness and Age
|
Risky actions in relationships |
Perception of seriousness |
|
Risky actions in relationships |
Pearson correlation |
1 |
-.205** |
Sig. (bilateral) |
|
<0.001 |
|
N |
2383 |
467 |
|
Perception of seriousness |
Pearson correlation |
-.205** |
1 |
Sig. (bilateral) |
<0.001 |
|
|
N |
467 |
481 |
|
**The correlation is significant at the level of 0.01 (bilateral). |
Table 9. Correlation between actions and the perception of seriousness
Table 9 shows a weak negative correlation (-0.205) between the actions related to relationships and the perception of seriousness, indicating that as risky actions increase, the perception of seriousness tends to decrease. This relationship is statistically significant at a level of 0.01, with a bilateral significance of < 0.001.
Finally, Table 10 shows the correlation between risky actions and perception, taking into account the age variable.
The weak negative correlation (-0.189) between risky actions and the perception of their seriousness suggests that, as the actions increase, the perception of the seriousness of the situation tends to decrease, even when controlling for the age variable. This relationship is statistically significant (p < 0.001).
Control variables |
Risky actions in relationships |
Perception of seriousness |
||
Age |
Risky actions in relationships |
Correlation |
1.000 |
-0.189 |
Significance (bilateral) |
. |
<0.001 |
||
gl |
0 |
464 |
||
Perception of seriousness |
Correlation |
-0.189 |
1.000 |
|
Significance (bilateral) |
<0.001 |
. |
||
gl |
464 |
0 |
Table 10. Correlation between actions and perception, taking age into account
4. Conclusions
The use of social media by the adolescent population constitutes a multi-faceted phenomenon that has as many potential benefits as it does significant risks. Nowadays, adolescents use these digital platforms as a means of socialization and expressing their personal interests, which can contribute to the consolidation of their identity and provide valuable emotional support, particularly in the context of communities with shared interests.
However, the adolescent community is exposed to a series of risks inherent to social media and the Internet, such as interaction with unknown users, cyber-bullying behavior, online isolation and exposure to inappropriate or harmful contents.
According to the statistical results of this study, the correlations indicate that as age increases, risky actions also tend to increase (p < 0.001). However, it cannot be concluded that a significant relationship exists between age and the perception of seriousness (p = 0.052). In a similar manner, as risky actions increase, the perception of their seriousness tends to decrease, even when controlling for the age variable (p < 0.001). This finding, viewed from an innovative perspective, suggests that age is not affecting the relationship between risky actions and the perception of seriousness in adolescents.
These data are related to the analysis of the perception of seriousness. The results establish that the increase in the perception of seriousness is associated with a reduction in the performance of risky behaviors. We could say that the perception of seriousness is a variable that plays a crucial role in the modulation of risky behaviors in the adolescent population in the digital environment. These results agree with the findings of Livingstone and Helsper (2010), who propose that the perception of seriousness online among adolescents is inversely related to the frequency with which they are exposed to risky contents. In other words, being familiar with risky actions leads to their naturalization and thus reduces the perception of seriousness.
Along the same lines, previous research (Marwick & Boyd, 2014; Zhu et al., 2021) suggests that the dynamics established on the social media call into question concepts such as trust, privacy, security, respect for the rights of others and healthy coexistence. All these dynamics generate inertia between the adolescent community that go against the perception of the seriousness of their actions and naturalize actions such as cyber-bullying, accepting friendship from someone they met on the Internet or passivity by those who are witnesses to bullying.
Likewise, the results in this regard highlight the importance of developing educational and preventive strategies focused on increasing awareness of the seriousness of online risks, especially among older adolescents, who tend to engage in more risky behaviors, which coincides with other studies, such as that by Cuervo-Sánchez and Etxague (2023). The increased awareness of the seriousness of the risk is one of the most novel aspects of the results of this research. While previous research (Gómez-Tabares & Correa‑Duque, 2022; Lareki et al., 2024) has focused to a greater extent on risky behaviors, this research delves deeper into the importance of addressing and raising awareness of the perception of the seriousness of these risks.
On the other hand, these results suggest that educational strategies in digital literacy could improve the perception of the seriousness of the risks, promoting the development of critical thinking about the relationships that are established online and the contents that are produced and consumed. This includes questioning the underlying interests of the messages, the ideologies that are behind them and their subjective interpretation. At the same time, these results can be innovative in the area of the responsible and critical use of the Internet and digital devices, as they suggest how important it is for educational interventions to seek to increase the perception of risk to reduce risky behaviors. In other words, this involves guiding students in a critical reflection on how their actions have implications in their environment and how both individual and collective well-being must be the guiding principle for their actions. This implies, for example, avoiding the normalization of violence on the Internet and fostering co-responsibility in the protection of privacy. Along these lines, the proper perception of the seriousness of the risks may help the adolescent population avoid certain actions, thus reducing situations of aggression or victimization and contributing to a better climate of coexistence in society.
In this sense, the data from this study indicate that adolescent boys engage in more risky actions, but girls tend to have a greater perception of their seriousness. These results agree with previous research (Cuervo‑Sánchez & Etxague, 2023; Marwick, 2021; Slonje & Smith, 2008) and with study data that show that people who engage in fewer risky actions are precisely the ones who have a greater perception of their seriousness. In other words, the need to work on the seriousness of their actions as a prevention strategy is reiterated, especially with adolescent boys.
Even though risky conducts are, as a whole, relatively uncommon, the main results reflect areas that require greater awareness, especially in relation to social exclusion (25.5% have excluded classmates on a few occasions) and passivity in the presence of bullying (27.2% indicate that they rarely intervene when someone is being bullied). Quite the opposite occurs with actions related to direct aggressions, such as threats and bullying, which are seen as more serious (50.4 % consider “threatening classmates” to be serious and 51.1% see “making comments that promote bullying” as serious).
The results obtained in this study regarding the passivity in situations of bullying agree with previous research that indicates the social permissiveness towards individuals who hold some type of power or who fall within the socially accepted norm (Marwick, 2021). They also agree with the findings related to the tendency to take a passive stance in situations of bullying, especially when the violence is not perceived as serious, or is supported by the idea of the virtual realm as a mere simulation that is not materialized in reality (Gómez-Tabares & Correa-Duque, 2022). This phenomenon contributes to the acceptance and perpetuation of bullying, by normalizing subtle aggressions and limiting intervention in situations which, while harmful, are interpreted as inoffensive.
Garandeau et al. (2023) indicate that the acknowledgment that observers play a role in events related to bullying has opened up new opportunities for interventions against bullying. In this way, our results also suggest that, instead of focusing intervention efforts exclusively on the aggressors and victims, it could prove promising to focus on modifying the perceptions and attitudes of the observers to encourage them to actively defend their victimized classmates. Programs like KiVa, the strategies of which include support for the actions and active support of spectators towards victimized classmates, have proven effective in reducing school bullying in different countries (Huitsing, Lodder, Browne, Oldenburg, Van der Ploeg & Veenstra, 2020; Kärnä, Voeten, Little, Poskiparta, Kaljonen & Salmivalli, 2011). From this perspective, both the perception of seriousness and the response to critical situations must be a focal point in the educational process. While educational interventions should take on various forms, it is crucial for them to promote the development of critical thinking and competencies before the action occurs, or without it being necessary for a violent situation to occur. This implies fostering reflection in symbolic spaces, promoting the expression of ideas and facilitating debate among peers, with the goal of preventing learning from depending exclusively on the experience after a risky behavior.
In conclusion, in order to counteract risky behaviors and prevent their repetition, it is essential to promote a response based on co-responsibility and the commitment to collective well-being, instead of opting for fear or indifference. For this reason, it is important to involve all educational agents, particularly those who adopt a posture of false neutrality. On a similar note, it is also important to investigate specific interventions to improve the perception of the seriousness of these behaviors, thus reducing the risks in vulnerable populations and fostering a culture of respect, inclusion and solidarity action, in digital as well as real environments.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
This work was carried out with assistance received from the call for the granting of aid to contracts Ramón y Cajal 2022 (RYC2022-035979-I), co-financed by MICIU/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 and by the FSE+ and the call for the granting of aid to research groups of the University of the Basque Country/Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea (UPV/EHU) (Ref. GIU 24-015).
References
Arab, L.E., & Díaz, G.A. (2015). Impacto de las redes sociales e internet en la adolescencia: aspectos positivos y negativos. Revista Médica Clínica Las Condes, 26(1), 7-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rmclc.2014.12.001
Brandt, S., & Carmichael, C. (2020). Does online support matter? The rela-tionship between online identity-related support, mattering, and well-being in sexual minority men. Computer in Human Behavior, 111, 106429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106429
Carras, M.C., Van Rooij, A.J., Van de Mheen, D., Musci, R., Xue, Q.L., & Mendelson, T. (2017). Video gaming in a hyperconnected world: A cross-sectional study of heavy gaming, problematic gaming symptoms, and online socializing in adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 68, 472-479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.060
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. https://doi.org/10.1016/C2013-0-10517-X
Cuervo-Sánchez, S.L., & Etxague, I. (2023). The four P’s on the Internet: Pornography, plagiarism, piracy and permission. Comunicar, 76, 85-96. https://doi.org/10.3916/C76-2023-07
Ellison, N.B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook “friends:” Social capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12(4), 1143-1168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x
Etxague-Goia, I. (2023) Adin txikikoen interneten eta sare sozialen erabilera eta hezkuntza-testuingurua. Kasu azterketa bat. Tesis doctoral. Universidad del País Vasco EHU/UPV. Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10810/66252
Garandeau, C.F., Turunen, T., Saarento-Zaprudin, S., & Salmivalli, C. (2023). Effects of the KiVa anti-bullying program on defending behavior: Investigating individual-level mechanisms of change. Journal of School Psychology, 99, 101226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2023.101226
Gómez-Tabares, A.S., & Correa-Duque, M.C. (2022). El efecto de la desvinculación moral sobre el acoso escolar, el ciberacoso y otros comportamientos disruptivos en niños (as) y adolescentes. Una revisión de la literature. Psicogente, 25(48), 204-230. https://doi.org/10.17081/psico.25.48.5455
Huh, J., Delorme, D.E., & Reid, L.N. (2006). Perceived third-person effects and consumer attitudes on preventing and banning DTC advertising. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 40(1), 90-116. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6606.2006.00047.x
Huitsing, G., Lodder, G.M.A., Browne, W.J., Oldenburg, B., Van der Ploeg, R., & Veenstra, R. (2020). A large-scale replication of the effectiveness of the KiVa anti-bullying program: A randomized controlled trial in the Netherlands. Prevention Science, 21(5), 627-638. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-020-01116-4
Kärnä, A., Voeten, M., Little, T.D., Poskiparta, E., Kaljonen, A., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). A large-scale evaluation of the KiVa anti-bullying program: Grades 4–6. Child Development, 82(1), 311-330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01557.x
Kumar, V.L., & Goldstein, M.A. (2020). Cyberbullying and Adolescents. Current Pediatrics Reports, 8, 86-92. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40124-020-00217-6
Lambe, L.J., Della-Cioppa, V., Hong, I.K., & Craig, W.M. (2019). Standing up to bullying: A social ecological review of peer defending in offline and online contexts. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 45, 51‑74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.05.007
Lareki, A., Fraga-Varela, F., & Martínez-de-Morentin, J.I. (2024). Adolescents and negligent social media use. Technology in Society, 78, 102623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techsoc.2024.102623
Livingstone, S., & Helsper, E. (2010). Balancing opportunities and risks in teenagers’ use of the internet: The role of online skills and internet self-efficacy. New Media & Society, 12(2), 309-329. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444809342697
Livingstone, S., & Stoilova, M. (2021). The 4Cs: Classifying Online Risk to Children. (CO:RE Short Report Series on Key Topics). Hamburg: Leibniz-Institut für Medienforschung | Hans-Bredow-Institut (HBI). CO:RE - Children Online: Research and Evidence. https://doi.org/10.21241/ssoar.71817
López-Sáez, M.Á., Pérez-Torres, V., Pastor, Y., Lobato-Rincón, L.L., Thomas-Currás, H., & Angulo‑Brunet, A. (2024). Uso de redes sociales en adolescentes: motivación, estrés de minorías y bienestar eudaimónico. Anales de Psicología / Annals of Psychology, 40(2), 272-279. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.556871
Lumsden, J. (2007). Online-questionnaire design guidelines. In Handbook of research on electronic surveys and measurements (44-64). Hershey. https://doi.org/10.4018/978-1-59140-792-8.ch005
Malhotra, N. (2008). Investigación de Mercados. Pearson Education. https://bit.ly/3YVflmU
Marco, J.H., & Tormo-Irun, M.P. (2018). Cyber Victimization Is Associated with Eating Disorder Psychopathology in Adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00987
Martínez-de-Morentin, J.I., Lareki, A., & Altuna, J. (2021). Risks associated with posting content on the social media. IEEE Revista Iberoamericana de Tecnologías del Aprendizaje, 16, 77-83. https://doi.org/10.1109/RITA.2021.3052655
Martínez-De-Morentin, J.I., Lareki, A., Altuna, J., & Amenabar, N. (2018). Cuestionario “Anomia digitala. Teknologia Digitalen erabilera eta jokaera desegokiak / Anomia digital. Uso de las tecnologías digitales y comportamientos inadecuados”. Universidad del País Vasco / Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea (UPV/EHU).
Marwick, A.E. (2021). Morally Motivated Networked Harassment as Normative Reinforcement. Social Media + Society, 7(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/20563051211021378
Marwick, A.E., & Boyd, D. (2014). Privacidad en red: cómo los adolescentes negocian el contexto en las redes sociales. New Media & Society, 16(7), 1051-1067. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444814543995
Norman, K.L., Friedman, Z., Norman, K., & Stevenson, R. (2001). Navigational issues in the design of online self-administered questionnaires. Behaviour & Information Technology, 20(1), 37-45. https://doi.org/10.1080/01449290010021764
Ortega, R., Elipe, P., Mora-Merchán, A., Calmaestra, J., & Vega, E. (2009). The emotional impact on victims of traditional bullying and cyberbullying: A study of Spanish adolescents. Zeitschrift fűr Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 97-204. https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409.217.4.197
Pew Research Center. (2018). A majority of teens have experienced some form of cyberbullying. https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2018/09/27/a-majority-of-teens-have-experienced-some-form-of-cyberbullying/
Slonje, R., & Smith, P. K. (2008). Cyberbullying: Another Main Type of Bullying? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 147-154. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2007.00611.x
Svensson, R., Johnson, B., & Olsson, A. (2022). Does gender matter? The association between different digital media activities and adolescent well-being. BMC Public Health, 22, 273. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-022-12670-7
Taylor, D.A., & Altman, I. (1987). Communication in interpersonal relationships: Social penetration processes. In Roloff, M.E., & Miller, G.R. (Eds.), Interpersonal processes: New directions in communication research (257-277). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Wang, X., Gao, Y., & Chen, B. (2023). Gender differences in cyberbullying tolerance, traditional bullying victimization and their associations with cyberbullying perpetration among Beijing adolescents. Child & Family Social Work, 30(1), 15-25. https://doi.org/10.1111/cfs.13079
White, J.A., Carey, L.M., & Daily, K.A. (2001). Web-based instrumentation in educational survey research. WebNet Journal, 3(1), 46-50.
Wright, M.F., & Wachs, S. (2020). Adolescents’ Cyber Victimization: The Influence of Technologies, Gender, and Gender Stereotype Traits. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(4), 1293. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17041293
Zani, A.A.A., Norman, A.A., & Ghani, N.A. (2022). Motivating factors to self-disclosure on social media: A systematic mapping. IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication, 65(3), 370-391. https://doi.org/10.1109/TPC.2022.3184428
Zhu, C., Huang, S., Evans, R., & Zhang, W. (2021) Cyberbullying Among Adolescents and Children: A Comprehensive Review of the Global Situation, Risk Factors, and Preventive Measures. Front. Public Health, 9, 634909. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2021.634909
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License
Journal of Technology and Science Education, 2011-2025
Online ISSN: 2013-6374; Print ISSN: 2014-5349; DL: B-2000-2012
Publisher: OmniaScience